Yiling Huo
1st Chandler House Symposium
"This morning I went to Starbucks to buy ... coffee"
"I eat rice"
DeLong et al. (2005)
However: Nieuwland et al. (2018) (N=356)
Lexical tone: pitch contour that encodes meaning.
ma1 "mother", ma2 "hemp", ma3 "horse", ma4 "scold"
ma1ma1-ma4-ma3 "Mom scolds the horse."
Tone sandhi: lexical tone can undergo change triggered by neighboring linguistic environments.
The yi sandhi: numeral yi1 "one"
yi4tian1, yi4tiao2, yi4ba3, yi2ge4
The T3 sandhi: numeral liang3 "two"
xiao3gou3 → xiao2gou3
The yi sandhi ("yi"):
The T3 sandhi ("liang"):
The yi sandhi ("yi"):
The T3 sandhi ("liang"):
Can listeners use tone sandhi to generate predictions?
Perhaps, but this effect is not always replicated.
Can listeners use tone sandhi to generate predictions?
Perhaps, but this effect is not always replicated.
Can listener use tone sandhi to detect prediction errors / revise their predictions?
The yi sandhi ("yi"):
The T3 sandhi ("liang"):
Can listeners use tone sandhi to generate predictions?
Perhaps, but this effect is not always replicated.
Can listener use tone sandhi to detect prediction errors / revise their predictions?
No evidence for revising prediction using tone sandhi.
Phonology's role in prediction may be limited.
We see that many aspects of language are involved in prediction. Why not phonology?
We see that many aspects of language are involved in prediction. Why not phonology?
Processing time hypothesis:
We see that many aspects of language are involved in prediction. Why not phonology?
The 'Filter' hypothesis:
Altmann, G. T., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73(3), 247-264.
Chow, W. Y., Smith, C., Lau, E., & Phillips, C. (2016). A “bag-of-arguments” mechanism for initial verb predictions. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(5), 577-596.
DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2005). Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature neuroscience, 8(8), 1117-1121.
Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). A rose by any other name: Long-term memory structure and sentence processing. Journal of memory and Language, 41(4), 469-495.
Ito, A., Corley, M., Pickering, M. J., Martin, A. E., & Nieuwland, M. S. (2016). Predicting form and meaning: Evidence from brain potentials. Journal of Memory and Language, 86, 157-171.
Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T., & Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and language, 49(1), 133-156.
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307(5947), 161-163.
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2007). Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological science, 18(3), 193-198.
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2010). Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of memory and language, 63(4), 447-464.
Nieuwland, M. S., Politzer-Ahles, S., Heyselaar, E., Segaert, K., Darley, E., Kazanina, N., ... & Huettig, F. (2018). Large-scale replication study reveals a limit on probabilistic prediction in language comprehension. ELife, 7, e33468.
Shun, L., Chen, X., & Wang, S. (2023). The involvement of phonological information during spoken language prediction: evidence based on Chinese tone sandhi. OSF Preprints.